The Sleeping Habits of Preschool Children Are Related to Temperament, Behavior, and Social Responsiveness

Article information

Sleep Med Res. 2013;4(1):23-27
Publication date (electronic) : 2013 June 30
doi : https://doi.org/10.17241/smr.2013.4.1.23
1Department of Psychiatry, Asan Medical Center, University of Ulsan College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
2Department of Psychiatry, Ulsan University Hospital, University of Ulsan College of Medicine, Ulsan, Korea
3Graduate School of Professional Therapeutic Technology, Seoul Womens University, Seoul, Korea
Correspondence: Seockhoon Chung, MD, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Asan Medical Center, University of Ulsan College of Medicine, 88 Olympic-ro 43-gil, Songpa-gu, Seoul 138-736, Korea, Tel +82-2-3010-3411, Fax +82-2-485-8381, E-mail schung@amc.seoul.kr
Received 2014 July 11; Revised 2014 September 15; Accepted 2014 September 24.

Abstract

Background and Objective

Children’s sleep habits are important for their health and development. Here, we investigated the relationship between sleep habits, behavior, personality, and social responsiveness in children.

Methods

A total of 38 preschool children were assessed using the Korean Child Behavior Checklist (K-CBCL), Junior Temperament and Character Inventory (JTCI), Children’s Sleep Habit Questionnaire (CSHQ), and Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS).

Results

Harm avoidance according to JTCI was positively correlated with bedtime resistance (r = 0.34; p = 0.04) and sleep anxiety on the CSHQ (r = 0.43; p < 0.01). Emotional lability on the K-CBCL was significantly associated with parasomnias (r = 0.38; p = 0.02), while lower social cognition and social communication scores on the SRS were associated with sleep anxiety (r = −0.34; p = 0.03) and parasomnias (r = −0.38; p = 0.01). Path analysis showed that among the JTCI subscales, both harm avoidance and persistence were significantly associated with social cognition.

Conclusions

Clinicians should pay particular attention to psychological components as potential contributors to sleep habits when managing sleep problems in preschool children.

INTRODUCTION

Proper sleep is an important factor associated with the physical and mental health of children. Numerous studies have reported poor sleep quality to be related to school performance, physical/mental development, behavior, and cognition.1,2 Children’s sleep may be influenced by a variety of factors, such as health status, stressful events, home sleeping environment, sleeping conditions, parental relationships, and usage of media devices.35 Psychological factors, including the child’s mood state, anxiety, depressive symptoms, and psychiatric illnesses such as generalized anxiety disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder can also negatively affect the quality of children’s sleep and sleep problems.6,7

Temperament is a characteristic of an individual’s nature, and its use originates from the ancient psychobiological concept of the four humors. Temperament can affect a child’s mood, emotional regulation, adaptability, activities, and inhibition.810 In addition, various mental and physical illnesses have been reported to be predisposed or predicted by temperament.11,12 However, few studies have evaluated the association between temperament and sleep problems in children. When a child’s emotions, mood status, or physical condition affects sleep problems, we can only speculate that the temperament of the child is related to the sleep problem.

Sleep problems can impact behavior and social relatedness. It is well known that sleep disturbances, such as sleep-disordered breathing, can make children hyperactive or inattentive.13 Good sleep can help children to improve their social skills and peer acceptance.14 We hypothesized that a child’s temperament may influence sleep disturbances and sleep problems, and that in turn, sleep problems may change the behavior and social relatedness of the children. However, these associations remain uncertain. The aim of this study was to investigate the association between temperament, sleep problems, and behavioral problems in a cohort of children.

METHODS

Participants

This study was a continuation of our previous study that investigated parenting stress,15 and specifically focused on the sleep habits of preschool children. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of all the children who participated in this study, and the protocol was approved by the institutional review board of the Asan Medical Center. In total, 38 preschool children between 3–6 years of age were enrolled. Children who met the following criteria were excluded: 1) currently using any psychotropic agents or sleeping pills; 2) diagnosed with any major psychiatric disorders, such as major depressive disorder, anxiety disorder, or any behavioral problems; and 3) the presence of any sleep disorders, including primary insomnia, circadian rhythm sleep disorder, restless legs syndrome, periodic limb movements during sleep, or sleep-disordered breathing. All developmental and psychiatric histories were examined by a child psychiatrist. The presence of sleep disorders was assessed using parental reports according to the diagnostic criteria of the International Classification of Sleep Disorders-2nd edition.

Measures

Temperament was assessed using the children’s adjusted Junior Temperament and Character Inventory (JTCI).16 In the present study, the parent-reported version of the JTCI was used. Using this scale, the child’s personality consists of two major constructs: temperament and character. Temperament is broken down into four dimensions: novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, and persistence. Character consists of the following three dimensions: self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence. The validated Korean version of the JTCI was used herein.17 Children’s sleep disturbances and sleep problems were assessed using the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ).18 The CSHQ is a sleep questionnaire for children between 4–12 years of age that was originally developed to screen for the most common sleep problems. The CSHQ focuses on sleep problems in terms of bedtime resistance, delays in sleep onset, sleep duration, sleep anxiety, night wakings, parasomnias, sleep-disordered breathing, and daytime sleepiness. A high total score on the CSHQ indicates poor sleep quality.

The Korean Child Behavior Checklist (K-CBCL)19–the Korean version of the Child Behavior Checklist20–was used to assess each child’s behavior as reported by their parents. The K-CBCL consists of various subscales: withdrawal, somatic complaints, anxiety, depression, thought problems, social problems, attention problems, delinquent behaviors, aggressive behaviors, internalizing problems, externalizing problems, and the total score. In addition, the emotional lability subscale was also included on the K-CBCL. The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS)21 was also used to assess social interactions. This includes social awareness, social cognition, social communication, social motivation, and autistic mannerism.

Statistical Analysis

The Student’s t-test was used to compare clinical characteristics and rating scale scores between boys and girls. Pearson’s correlation analysis was performed to determine the associations between rating scale scores. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 for two-tailed tests. The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 12.0K, IBML; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used to conduct all statistical tests. The SPSS Amos (version 7.0, IBM) was used to perform the path analyses.

RESULTS

Among the 38 enrolled preschoolers, the mean age was 4.4 ± 0.7 years and 20 participants were boys (53%). There were no significant differences between boys and girls in terms of age and the various items on the JTCI, K-CBCL, CSHQ, and SRS rating scales, except for night wakings on the CHSQ (boys: 3.3 ± 0.6; girls: 3.9 ± 0.8; p = 0.01) and social cognition on the SRS (boys: 51.3 ± 6.3; girls: 56.6 ± 7.9; p = 0.03). Among the temperament and character domains, a high level of harm avoidance was correlated with bedtime resistance (r = 0.34; p = 0.04) and sleep anxiety (r = 0.43; p < 0.01) on the CSHQ (Table 1). High emotional lability on the K-CBCL was significantly related with parasomnia on the CSHQ (r = 0.38; p = 0.02). Among items related to social responsiveness, low social cognition on the SRS was correlated with sleep anxiety on the CSHQ (r = −0.34; p = 0.03), and low social communication on the SRS was associated with parasomnia on the CSHQ (r = −0.41; p = 0.01) (Table 2).

Correlations between sleep habits, temperament, and character in children

Correlations between children’s sleep habits and social responsiveness

After careful consideration of the previous results and the construction of several models, we decided to include the following subscales/items in our structural equation model: harm avoidance, self-directedness, persistence, novelty seeking, sleep anxiety, social cognition, autistic mannerisms, and delinquent behavior. All JTCI subscales were arranged as covariates to each other. Harm avoidance and self-directedness were considered as indicators of sleep anxiety, while self-directedness, persistence, and novelty seeking were considered as indicators of autistic mannerisms. Sleep anxiety and persistence were considered to be indicators of social cognition, which in turn, along with autistic mannerisms, was considered as an indicator of delinquent behavior (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Path analysis of the relationships in children between various temperament and character subscales, sleep anxiety, social cognition, autistic mannerisms, and delinquent behavior. Model Indices: χ2 = 7.85, df = 13, p = 0.85, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = 1.00, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) < 0.01, and Normed Fit Index (NFI) = 0.86. Rectangles represent observed variables. Numbers next to the single-headed arrows correspond to standardized regression weights. *p < 0.05. p < 0.01. The Junior Temperament and Character Inventory (JTCI), Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ), Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), and the Korean Child Behavior Checklist (K-CBCL) were used, and only the subscales/items presented in this figure were entered and analyzed as variables. All JTCI subscales presented here were arranged as covariates of each other, which is not shown in this diagram.

The overall applicability of a path model can be assessed according to its goodness-of-fit indexes. Although there are no clear-cut indications, the chi-square value, Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Normed Fit Index (NFI), and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) are some of the most widely acknowledged values; moreover, insignificant chi-square values, CFI > 0.9, NFI > 0.95, and RMSEA < 0.08 are usually considered as adequate cutoffs.22 The indices for the current study were calculated as follows: χ2 = 7.85, df = 13, p = 0.85, CFI = 1.00, NFI = 0.86, and RMSEA < 0.01, all of which were considered indications of a satisfactory, if not perfect, fit. Harm avoidance was associated with sleep anxiety (β = 0.18; p < 0.01), which in turn demonstrated a comparatively strong association with social cognition (β = −1.50; p = 0.01). Persistence was also strongly associated with social cognition (β = −1.69; p = 0.03). The association between social cognition and delinquent behavior, on the other hand, was insignificant. Because these results were based on information obtained using a cross-sectional survey, the associations described in this paper are only theoretical at this stage.

DISCUSSION

In the current study, we observed sleep habits and sleep problems in preschool children to be associated with harm avoidance, emotional lability, and social cognition or communication. Path analysis showed that harm avoidance, sleep anxiety, and social cognition may be sequentially associated with each other, and that both persistence and sleep anxiety may be important factors determining social cognition. Harm avoidance is a temperament trait that is associated with aversion to non-rewarding stimuli, high anticipation of threats, and negative emotions during stress. It has also been reported to be related to anxiety disorders in children.23 In the current analyses, harm avoidance was found to be related to bedtime resistance and sleep anxiety. According to our previous study on adult patients with psycho-physiological insomnia, harm avoidance is also significantly associated with the severity of insomnia, anxiety, and dysfunctional sleep-related cognitions.24 Individuals with high levels of harm avoidance tend to internalize their feelings and emotions and repress conflicts.25 This internal stress can influence both sleep problems and sleep disturbance.

In the present study, emotional lability on the K-CBCL test was significantly related to parasomnia. Emotional lability is a sub-scale only on the K-CBCL, and refers to rapid, often exaggerated changes in mood, where strong emotions or feelings occur. In children, it pertains to those who easily cry, become jealous, and throw temper tantrums. It is widely known that parasomnia may be related to anxiety, traumatic injury, stress, and the loss of loved ones.26,27 Strong emotional tension can trigger non-rapid eye movement parasomnia in children. Here, social cognition and social communication were found to be associated with sleep anxiety and parasomnia. Although we cannot yet confirm a causal relationship, some studies have reported that sleep can influence or be affected by social relationships.14 It can be speculated that sleep disturbance may affect social relationships, but it is more feasible at this stage that emotional stress due to decreased social competence may influence sleep.

The structural equation model herein revealed a relationship between harm avoidance and social cognition that may be mediated by sleep anxiety. Previous studies have reported high harm avoidance in autistic patients,28,29 but we were unable to find any previous studies specific to harm avoidance and social cognition. As mentioned above, individuals with a high degree of harm avoidance are prone to internalizing their feelings, which in turn may influence sleep problems. It can also be hypothesized that repetitive sleep problems, particularly sleep anxiety, may decrease self-confidence. Because social cognition can be thought of as an ability which requires experience and at least some level of self-confidence–i.e., the correct interpretation of social situations would be impossible if one repeatedly second-guessed oneself– it could be inferred that children with high harm avoidance tend to demonstrate more sleeping problems, especially sleep anxiety, and that harm avoidance and repeated sleep anxiety together cause a decline in social cognition.

Persistence was also found to be associated with social cognition in path analysis. Although we were unable to find any earlier studies that directly assessed persistence and social cognition, we did find one that reported persistence in association with substance use disorders in autistic patients.30 If social cognition is considered as a thought process, then it can be argued that persistence is needed to follow that process to its conclusion. As already mentioned, these results are theoretical, but we believe sleep anxiety plays a role that deserves further attention, as this was somewhat unexpected.

This study had several limitations of note. First, our patients did not undergo nocturnal polysomnography in order to evaluate their current sleep status. This means that we could not completely exclude the presence of sleep-disordered breathing or periodic limb movements. Second, the sample size of this study may have been too small to detect significant differences in groups. Finally, the subjects enrolled in this study were 3 to 6 years, and we used the parent-rated JTCI for analysis, which can be applied to those 5 to 14 years old.31 The discrepancy may have affected the results. In addition, we could not use the validated Korean version of the CSHQ and SRS in this study, as the scales have not yet been validated in Korean.

In conclusion, we observed herein that temperament, social relations, and behavior are associated with sleep problems and sleep disturbance in children.

Notes

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no financial conflicts of interest.

References

1. Magee CA, Gordon R, Caputi P. Distinct developmental trends in sleep duration during early childhood. Pediatrics 2014;133:e1561–7.
2. Michels N, Clays E, De Buyzere M, Vanaelst B, De Henauw S, Sioen I. Children’s sleep and autonomic function: low sleep quality has an impact on heart rate variability. Sleep 2013;36:1939–46.
3. Spilsbury JC, Babineau DC, Frame J, Juhas K, Rork K. Association between children’s exposure to a violent event and objectively and subjectively measured sleep characteristics: a pilot longitudinal study. J Sleep Res 2014;23:585–94.
4. Meltzer LJ, Mindell JA. Relationship between child sleep disturbances and maternal sleep, mood, and parenting stress: a pilot study. J Fam Psychol 2007;21:67–73.
5. McKenna JJ, Mosko SS. Sleep and arousal, synchrony and independence, among mothers and infants sleeping apart and together (same bed): an experiment in evolutionary medicine. Acta Paediatr Suppl 1994;397:94–102.
6. Shanahan L, Copeland WE, Angold A, Bondy CL, Costello EJ. Sleep problems predict and are predicted by generalized anxiety/depression and oppositional defiant disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2014;53:550–8.
7. Gruber R. ADHD, anxiety and sleep: a window to understanding the interplay between sleep, emotional regulation and attention in children? Behav Sleep Med 2014;12:84–7.
8. Thomas A, Chess S. Temperament and development New York: Brunner/Mazel; 1977.
9. Santos RG, Quinonez R. Child temperament is as strongly associated with early childhood caries (ECC) as poor feeding practices: positive temperament appears protective, negative temperament may increase ECC risk. J Evid Based Dent Pract 2014;14:85–8.
10. Tan PZ, Armstrong LM, Cole PM. Relations between Temperament and Anger Regulation over Early Childhood. Soc Dev 2013;22(4)10.1111/j.1467-9507.2012.00674.x.
11. Laceulle OM, Ormel J, Vollebergh WA, van Aken MA, Nederhof E. A test of the vulnerability model: temperament and temperament change as predictors of future mental disorders-the TRAILS study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2014;55:227–36.
12. Kerekes N, Brändström S, Lundström S, Råstam M, Nilsson T, Anckarsäter H. ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, temperament, and character: phenotypical associations and etiology in a Swedish childhood twin study. Compr Psychiatry 2013;54:1140–7.
13. Chervin RD, Ruzicka DL, Giordani BJ, Weatherly RA, Dillon JE, Hodges EK, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing, behavior, and cognition in children before and after adenotonsillectomy. Pediatrics 2006;117:e769–78.
14. Vaughn BE, Elmore-Staton L, Shin N, El-Sheikh M. Sleep as a Support for Social Competence, Peer Relations, and Cognitive Functioning in Preschool Children. Behav Sleep Med 2014. [Epub ahead of print].
15. Park J, Chung S, Kim HW. The relationship of the parenting stress with child’s characteristics in preschool children. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011;22:236–45.
16. Luby JL, Svrakic DM, McCallum K, Przybeck TR, Cloninger CR. The Junior Temperament and Character Inventory: preliminary validation of a child self-report measure. Psychol Rep 1999;84(3 Pt 2):1127–38.
17. Lyoo IK, Han CH, Lee SJ, Yune SK, Ha JH, Chung SJ, et al. The reliability and validity of the junior temperament and character inventory. Compr Psychiatry 2004;45:121–8.
18. Owens JA, Spirito A, McGuinn M. The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep 2000;23:1043–51.
19. Oh KJ, Lee HR, Hong KE, Ha EH. Korea child behavior checklist. Seoul: Jung-Ang Juk-Sung Publisher; 1997.
20. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. Manual for the child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile. Burlington: University of Vermont; 1983.
21. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social Responsive Scale (SRS) Manual Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2005.
22. Hooper D, Coughlan J, Mullen MR. Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. Electron J Bus Res Method 2008;6:53–60.
23. Cho SC, Jung SW, Kim BN, Hwang JW, Shin MS, Kim JW, et al. Temperament and character among Korean children and adolescents with anxiety disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009;18:60–4.
24. Park JH, An H, Jang ES, Chung S. The influence of personality and dysfunctional sleep-related cognitions on the severity of insomnia. Psychiatry Res 2012;197:275–9.
25. Kales A, Caldwell AB, Soldatos CR, Bixler EO, Kales JD. Biopsychobehavioral correlates of insomnia. II. Pattern specificity and consistency with the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Psychosom Med 1983;45:341–56.
26. Kotagal S. Parasomnias of childhood. Curr Opin Pediatr 2008;20:659–65.
27. Hoban TF. Sleep disorders in children. Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2013;19(1 Sleep Disorders):185–98.
28. Soderstrom H, Rastam M, Gillberg C. Temperament and character in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism 2002;6:287–97.
29. Anckarsäter H, Stahlberg O, Larson T, Hakansson C, Jutblad SB, Niklasson L, et al. The impact of ADHD and autism spectrum disorders on temperament, character, and personality development. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:1239–44.
30. Sizoo B, van den Brink W, Gorissen van Eenige M, van der Gaag RJ. Personality characteristics of adults with autism spectrum disorders or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with and without substance use disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis 2009;197:450–4.
31. Washington University School of Medicine. St. Louis, MO: Washington University School of Medicine; [cited 2013 Sep 8]. Available from: http://psychobiology.wustl.edu/tci/for-researchers/tci-in-research/81.

Article information Continued

Fig. 1.

Path analysis of the relationships in children between various temperament and character subscales, sleep anxiety, social cognition, autistic mannerisms, and delinquent behavior. Model Indices: χ2 = 7.85, df = 13, p = 0.85, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = 1.00, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) < 0.01, and Normed Fit Index (NFI) = 0.86. Rectangles represent observed variables. Numbers next to the single-headed arrows correspond to standardized regression weights. *p < 0.05. p < 0.01. The Junior Temperament and Character Inventory (JTCI), Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ), Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), and the Korean Child Behavior Checklist (K-CBCL) were used, and only the subscales/items presented in this figure were entered and analyzed as variables. All JTCI subscales presented here were arranged as covariates of each other, which is not shown in this diagram.

Table 1.

Correlations between sleep habits, temperament, and character in children

CSHQ subscales Junior Temperament and Character Inventory

NS HA RD P SD C ST
Bedtime resistance 0.13 0.34* 0.30 −0.02 −0.06 −0.04 −0.20
Sleep onset delay 0.01 0.14 −0.22 −0.27 0.11 −0.11 −0.16
Sleep duration −0.19 0.31 −0.23 −0.19 0.08 −0.10 −0.17
Sleep anxiety 0.19 0.43 0.29 −0.07 −0.29 −0.04 −0.13
Night wakings 0.08 0.30 −0.08 −0.07 −0.26 0.11 0.03
Parasomnias 0.14 −0.03 0.07 −0.03 −0.17 −0.02 −0.30
Sleep disordered breathing −0.06 −0.01 −0.17 −0.03 −0.21 0.21 −0.14
Daytime sleepiness −0.04 0.03 −0.31 0.01 0.24 −0.18 −0.30
*

p < 0.05.

p < 0.01.

CSHQ: Children,s Sleep Habits Questionnaire, NS: novelty seeking, HA: harm avoidance, RD: reward dependence, P: persistence, SD: self-directedness, C: cooperativeness, ST: self-transcendence.

Table 2.

Correlations between children’s sleep habits and social responsiveness

CSHQ subscales Social Responsiveness Scale

Social awareness Social cognition Social communication Social motivation Autistic mannerism
Bedtime resistance −0.09 −0.26 −0.15 −0.02 0.09
Sleep onset delay 0.01 0.05 −0.07 −0.27 0.10
Sleep duration 0.08 0.20 0.04 −0.19 0.07
Sleep anxiety −0.06 −0.34* −0.20 −0.07 0.20
Night wakings −0.21 −0.07 −0.03 −0.07 0.19
Parasomnias −0.17 −0.27 −0.38* −0.03 0.01
Sleep disordered breathing 0.27 0.07 −0.06 −0.03 0.02
Daytime sleepiness −0.20 0.06 0.15 0.01 0.13
*

p < 0.05.

CSHQ: Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire.